Editorial Type:
Article Category: Research Article
 | 
Online Publication Date: Apr 22, 2025

Single-dose psychedelic enhances cognitive flexibility and reversal learning in mice weeks after administration

,
, and
Page Range: 1 – 7
DOI: 10.61373/pp025r.0002
Save
Download PDF

Psychedelic compounds have demonstrated remarkable therapeutic potential for treating neuropsychiatric disorders by promoting sustained neuroplasticity in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). Cognitive flexibility—the ability to adapt previously learned rules to novel situations—represents a critical PFC function that is frequently impaired in depression, PTSD, and neurodegenerative conditions. In this study, we demonstrate that a single administration of the selective serotonin 2A receptor agonist 25CN-NBOH produces significant, long-lasting improvements in cognitive flexibility in both male and female mice when measured 2–3 weeks posttreatment. Using a novel automated sequential learning paradigm, psychedelic-treated mice showed superior adaptability in rule reversal tasks compared to saline controls, as evidenced by enhanced poke efficiency, higher percentages of correct trials, and increased reward acquisition. These behavioral findings complement existing cellular research showing psychedelic-induced structural remodeling in the PFC and uniquely demonstrate sustained cognitive benefits persisting weeks after a single psychedelic dose. Our automated behavioral task provides a high-throughput method for evaluating cognitive flexibility effects of various psychedelic compounds, offering important implications for therapeutic applications in conditions characterized by cognitive rigidity, including depression, PTSD, and potentially Alzheimer's disease.

Introduction

Psychedelic drugs have been used to treat multiple neuropsychiatric disorders, including major depressive disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and substance use disorders (114). These neuropsychiatric disorders are precipitated by chronic stress, which leads to both structural and functional changes in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) in humans and rodents (1522). The therapeutic potential of psychedelics may be due to their ability to restore neural circuits damaged in these pathologies by boosting synaptic activity (2331).

The PFC contributes to the control of many cognitive functions, including working memory, memory retrieval, decision-making, and executive function (32, 33). One key aspect of executive function is the ability to apply previously learned rules to novel situations, also known as cognitive flexibility (33, 34). Flexibility disruptions are associated with neuropsychiatric disorders, such as depression and PTSD, as well as neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative disorders (34, 35). Cognitive flexibility has been examined using tasks such as the Flanker Task, Stroop Task, and the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task; however, these kinds of tasks are largely limited to humans (36). In contrast, most cognitive flexibility tasks for rodents can be classified as either attentional set-shifting paradigms, which involve the learning of two separate rules and associated cues, or reversal learning, which involves applying a learned rule to a reversed scenario (37, 38). Reversal learning is an effective method for studying cognitive flexibility in rodents, including mice (3840). Reversal learning paradigms can be extremely diverse, varying in the kind of tasks being taught to the number and timing of reversals involved in the paradigm. These details are critical when evaluating the existing literature's examination of cognitive flexibility through reversal learning.

25CN-NBOH is a psychedelic agent with high affinity and selectivity for the serotonin 2A (5-HT2A) receptor (4143). It has demonstrated psychedelic-like effects and is commonly used in the study of psychedelic mechanisms in rodents (4446). 25CN-NBOH has much stronger affinity for 5-HT2A receptors (50–100x higher affinity) in comparison to the closely related 5-HT2B and 5-HT2C receptors. Compared to 25CN-NBOH, other psychedelic drugs have a lower ratio of 5-HT2A to 5-HT2B or 5-HT2C affinity (4749). As 5-HT2A receptor activation, specifically, has previously been shown to be required for psychedelic-induced synaptogenesis that might contribute to behavioral changes, this high affinity and high selectivity 5-HT2A receptor agonist was chosen as the psychedelic drug for the present study of psychedelic effects on cognitive flexibility.

As single psychedelic administrations promote structural changes in the PFC that last for several weeks (24, 29, 31), here we asked whether a single psychedelic administration could also induce a weeks-long enhancement of flexible learning ability in mice. We conducted a reversal learning task in which female and male mice were administered a single dose of a psychedelic drug or saline and found enhanced performance on the reversal task which persists for at least 3 weeks after one psychedelic dose

Results

To determine whether psychedelic treatment induces long-lasting changes in flexible learning ability, we treated female and male mice with a single dose of the selective serotonin 2A (5-HT2A) receptor agonist 25CN-NBOH (4145) or saline via intraperitoneal injection. Following a waiting period of one day, light food restriction for 2 days, and 5 days of training with the Feeding Experimentation Device version 3 (FED3) device, we utilized a forward sequence learning protocol (Figure 1A). Mice learned to initiate a trial with a left poke and then had to poke right within the subsequent 30 s to receive a food pellet (Figure 1B). Following 6 days of 4 h/day forward protocol sessions, the required sequential poking pattern was reversed. For another 6 days of 4-h sessions, mice were then required to poke right and then poke left within 30 s to receive the food pellet (Figure 1C). This reversal of the experimental protocol is indicative of flexible learning: we measured the degree to which a mouse is able to adapt the previously learned 1 poke/hole sequence rule to a novel situation, which, in this case, was the reversed direction.

Figure 1.Figure 1.Figure 1.
Figure 1.Experimental timeline and overview. (A) Experimental timeline (65). (B) Schematic of the SEQFR2-forward protocol. Mice have to sequentially poke left and then right within 30 s to earn a reward pellet. (C) Schematic of the SEQFR2-reversal protocol. Mice now are required to poke right and then left within 30 s to get a reward pellet.

Citation: Psychedelics 2025; 10.61373/pp025r.0002

We found that psychedelic and saline-treated mice learned the forward task at similar rates, as reflected by the poke efficiency, which represents the proportion of pellets dispensed out of all pokes (Figure 2A), and the percentage of correct trials initiated out of all trials initiated (Figure 2B). While the change in forward learning poke efficiency and percentage correct was not affected by psychedelic treatment (poke efficiency: saline R2 = 0.25, NBOH R2 = 0.20; F(1,557)=.1721, P = 0.6784; percent correct: saline R2 = 0.16, NBOH R2 = 0.13; F(1,431) = .7771, P = 0.3785), the NBOH-treated group accumulated more reward pellets than the saline group (Figure 2C; saline: R2 = 0.18, NBOH: R2 = 0.27; F(1,620) = 7.513, P = 0.0063), indicating an increased initiation of trials per hour with the FED3 (Figure 2C), as the baseline and learning rates were similar between groups (Supplemental Figure 1).

Figure 2.Figure 2.Figure 2.
Figure 2.Single-dose psychedelic treatment induces a lasting reversal learning enhancement. (A) Group forward phase poke efficiency, with no significant effect of NBOH treatment. Each day has 4 points plotted corresponding to each hour of the 4-h per day sessions. (B) Group forward phase percentage of correct trials, indicating no significant effect of NBOH treatment. (C) NBOH treatment significantly increased the number of reward pellets dispensed during the forward phase. (D) NBOH treatment significantly increased poke efficiency during the reversal phase compared to saline injection, indicating enhanced cognitive flexibility. (E) NBOH increases the percentage of correct trials. (F) NBOH treatment significantly increased the number of reward pellets dispensed during the reversal phase. Shaded regions represent standard error of the mean (SEM), linear regressions shown in pink for NBOH and black for saline; ns, not significant; **p < 0.01; ****p < 0.0001.

Citation: Psychedelics 2025; 10.61373/pp025r.0002

Importantly, during the reversal phase, measured 15–20 days after the single injection, psychedelic treatment resulted in significantly increased learning ability. This is indicated by the increased the efficiency of nose pokes (Supplemental Figure 2; Figure 2D; saline: R2 = 0.11, NBOH: R2 = 0.32; F(1,528) = 21.91, P < 0.0001), the percent correct trials initiated (Figure 2E; saline: R2 = 0.11, NBOH: R2 = 0.23; F(1,401) = 6.629, P = 0.0104), and again by the higher total number of pellets obtained (Figure 2F; saline: R2 = 0.10, NBOH: R2 = 0.37; F(1,620) = 20.74, P < 0.0001).

We confirmed the robustness of these findings by conducting Welch's one-sided t-tests after calculating individual linear regression curves for each animal, and found that NBOH-treated mice (poke efficiency: M = 0.036, SD = 0.026; percent correct: M = 0.084, SD = 0.052), relative to saline treated mice (poke efficiency: M = 0.016, SD = 0.015; percent correct: M = 0.043, SD = 0.049) perform better on average in the reversal phase (reversal poke efficiency: t(17.40) = 2.29, p = 0.017, Cohen's d = 0.928; reversal percent correct: t(22.92) = 2.07, p = 0.0252, Cohen's d = 0.814). This was consistent with the aforementioned results. Also consistent with the above findings, the individual linear regression analysis showed that there were no significant differences in both the forward poke efficiency (t(22.58) = 0.380, p = 0.354, Cohen's d = 0.152) and forward percent correct (t(23.78)= −0.729, p = 0.763, Cohen's d=-0.286) metrics between NBOH (forward poke efficiency: M = 0.035, SD = 0.026; percent correct: M = 0.049, SD = 0.050) and saline-treated mice (forward poke efficiency: M = 0.031, SD = 0.026; percent correct: M = 0.063, SD = 0.053).

Finally, we considered sex as a biological variable to determine whether NBOH improved learning in both sexes. We found that, consistent with our sex-independent results (Figure 2), NBOH treatment did not affect poke efficiency during the forward phase (Female: F(1,334) = 0.986, P = .322; Male: F(1,219) = 0.004, P = 0.952), but significantly enhanced poke efficiency during the reversal phase (Figure 3; Female; F(1,319) = 16, P < .0001; Male: F(1,205) = 8.3, P = 0.0044). Thus, psychedelic treatment induced a weeks-long lasting enhancement of reversal learning in both male and female mice. When comparing male and female poke efficiency, we found that male mice treated with saline performed slightly better than female mice in both the forward and reversal phases (P = .0011; P = 0.0218), and male mice treated with NBOH performed better in the reversal phase than female mice (P = 0.0206).

Figure 3.Figure 3.Figure 3.
Figure 3.Lasting psychedelic enhancement of reversal learning ability in male and female mice. (Top) Forward and reversal phase changes to poke efficiency indicating NBOH treatment significantly improves reversal learning in female mice weeks after a single dose. (Bottom) Forward and reversal phase changes to poke efficiency in male mice indicating NBOH treatment significantly improves reversal learning weeks after a single dose. Shaded regions represent standard error of the mean (SEM), linear regressions shown in pink for NBOH and black for saline; ns, not significant; **p < 0.01; ****p < 0.0001.

Citation: Psychedelics 2025; 10.61373/pp025r.0002

Discussion

This study sought to examine the effects of a single psychedelic dose on flexible learning. We found that even 2–3 weeks after a single dose, NBOH significantly enhanced reversal learning ability. Poke efficiency, percentage of correct trials, and cumulative pellets dispensed were all improved during the reversal phase in mice that received NBOH compared to mice that received saline. Both male and female mice displayed improved learning during the reversal phase with psychedelic treatment, highlighting the therapeutic potential of psychedelic medicine to boost cognitive flexibility in both sexes. The estrous cycle can influence cognitive flexibility performance in rodents (50). The task design utilized in our study (6 days for each of the forward and reversal phases) encompasses more than the full length of a mouse estrous cycle (4–5 days) and the analysis metric utilized here examines changes in the slope of learning across the full 6 days, thus calculating learning rates over the course of at least one full estrous cycle. Future work will help to better understand the precise influence of estrous cycle on higher temporal resolution changes in reversal learning properties.

This study contrasts with previous preclinical psychedelic reversal learning studies in terms of drug administration timepoints (46, 5154). We administered the psychedelic or saline control 15 days before the start of the reversal protocol. Thus, our study focuses on the longer-term therapeutic effects of the psychedelic drug. It is important to distinguish such longer-term effects from immediate or short-lasting acute effects, that may be more related to the mind-altering impact of psychedelics and not to their longer-term therapeutic effects. In a two-choice visual discrimination task, 25CN-NBOH (1–2 mg/kg) was found to have no significant effects on reversal learning in mice when administered acutely, immediately before testing (46). Other previous rodent studies using attentional set-shifting and T-maze paradigms found impairment of flexible learning with acute administration of the psychedelics DOI (1 mg/kg) or 25CN-NBOH (1 mg/kg) on cognitive flexibility (53, 54). However, one study found acutely enhanced cognitive flexibility with acute psilocybin (1 mg/kg) in the same attentional set shifting paradigm that found impairment following administration of DOI (54). The differences in the acute effects of psychedelics on reversal learning may be due to the study design, discussed below, as well as a combination of drug and dose. DOI and 25CN-NBOH are much more potent (>10x) than psilocybin (41, 42, 4749). Concentration-dependent acute suppression of working memory (27, 5557) likely explains why the relatively less potent psychedelic psilocybin doesn't acutely impair behavioral performance at this dose. As the long-term effects of psychedelics on cognition are the effects that are more relevant therapeutically, it is important that future work continues to examine the sustained, in addition to acute, effects of psychedelics.

The current study also contrasts with previous studies in protocol design. We selected a reversal learning paradigm that is sufficiently complicated and somewhat easier to interpret compared to attentional set-shifting or T-maze paradigms. Two recent studies have also made use of reversal learning paradigms and have found long-term (study 1: 3 days; study 2: 14 days) positive effects of psychedelics on cognitive flexibility in female rats, but there are a few notable differences in behavioral protocols compared to the current study (51, 52). As opposed to conducting a sequential FR2-style task with only one reversal of the task, the 14-day long reversal study implemented an FR1 task that repeatedly reversed every 10 successful trials (51). While this study demonstrated that psilocybin increases the number of successes over time, it did not show if psilocybin improves accuracy, or if this is a function of increased trial initiations after psilocybin (51). Our FR2 style task with a single reversal after many days of training highlights the different effects psychedelics have on initial (forward) learning and reversal learning separately, which we would have otherwise been unable to do in a paradigm that frequently reverses. Our paradigm also likely results in fewer random successes that could inflate an animal's actual performance as we require precisely two sequential pokes in two separate holes. In addition, we conducted the task in both female and male mice. A similar study testing the long-term effects of DOI on reversal learning found that, depending on task structure, DOI has mixed effects on reversal learning ability (58). A week-long evaluation of initial learning after dosing appeared to assist in the enhancement of flexible learning, but if the animals were not exposed again to the task prior to reversal after dosing, DOI appears to have a negative effect on reversal learning ability. This finding suggests that further work needs to be done to evaluate what role practice following dosing has on cognitive flexibility.

In humans, psilocybin treatment has been found to improve cognitive flexibility up to 1 month after dosing (13, 14). However, these studies utilized a within-subject repeated measures design with no non-psilocybin control group (13), or with low dose psilocybin (1 mg) as the control group (14). Although promising, it is possible that behavioral performance was improved through familiarity with the task design rather than a direct result of the psychedelic treatment. It is currently unknown whether a single psychedelic dose would improve cognitive flexibility measured in a human study using independent measures.

PFC neurons have been shown to undergo spinogenesis and synaptogenesis after a single psychedelic administration through a pathway requiring 5-HT2A receptor activation (2331), but the precise mechanisms of 5-HT2A receptor induced flexible learning and how long these benefits can last are still unknown. Here, we use 25CN-NBOH, which is 50–100x more selective for 5-HT2A receptors over 5-HT2B and 5-HT2C receptors and has even weaker affinity for other 5-HT receptors (41, 42, 49). Future research into the long-term effects of other psychedelic drugs on cognitive flexibility will need to be conducted to examine whether psychedelics that target additional 5-HT receptor subtypes have similar long-lasting effects, or to determine whether the interaction with other 5-HT receptors abolishes the ability to enhance long-lasting flexibility. In addition, it remains unknown if non-hallucinogenic 5-HT2A receptor agonists such as 2-bromo-LSD, lisuride, and 6-fluoro-diethyltryptamine (5961), are also able to induce a lasting enhancement of flexible learning.

Psychedelic-mediated weeks-long enhancement of reversal learning ability allows for many further directions of research. Future studies will examine the effects that psychedelics have on mice across different ages. Additional studies will also determine the effects of different psychedelic drugs, dose levels, number of doses, or dose timing in this behavioral paradigm. While we did find an enhancement in a mouse model of cognitive flexibility with a 5-HT2A receptor agonist, we did not use 5-HT2A knockout mice to see if the absence of 5-HT2A receptors would cause any deficits in this behavior, or if psychedelics would improve this behavior without the engagement of 5-HT2A receptors.

A long-term positive psychedelic-induced enhancement of cognitive flexibility has several implications for future human psychedelic medicine, specifically for pathologies that involve deficits in executive function or synaptic loss. Cognitive flexibility is impaired in many disorders, including depression, PTSD, and Alzheimer's disease (AD) (6264). While clinical trials evaluating the impact of psychedelic medicine on depression and PTSD are already underway and have shown promising results (13, 14), psychedelics have not yet been used to try to treat cognitive flexibility in AD and related neurodegenerative diseases. Additional research using mouse models of AD would be important to mechanistically demonstrate that psychedelics can indeed boost flexibility in these models and to confirm that psychedelics can also boost long-term synaptic activity in brain regions related to cognitive flexibility, such as the PFC in these same preclinical models. The task design presented here will facilitate future studies that can address these and other questions. This will allow for an even greater mechanistic understanding of the relationship between psychedelic treatment and cognitive flexibility.

Materials and Methods

Animals and Behavioral Apparatus

The open source, programmable FED3 device was used for all behavioral experiments. We programmed the FED3 via Arduino to deliver a 10 mg reward pellet if an animal successfully pressed the correct sequence of nose poke holes (left-then-right or right-then-left within 30 s, depending on the forward or reversal phase of the task). The reward pellets used in this study were 10 mg Bio-Serv Dustless Precision Pellets in the chocolate flavor. The cages used with the FED3 devices were modified standard mouse housing cages, with holes drilled into the front of the cage and magnets affixed to the cage's front to allow the animal to interact with the reward well and nose poke holes and ensure the device stays flush to the side of the cage during data collection. Data were collected within the animals’ vivarium on a static shelf to minimize any effect of changing locations on stress and ensure ample room for both the cage and the device on the shelf. Each animal had their own experimentation cage and FED3 used for the duration of the experiment to minimize the stress of unfamiliar environment and odors. The animals’ vivarium ran on a reversed light cycle with lights off (dark phase) from 7:00 AM to 7:00 PM. Each day, data were collected from approximately 10:00 AM to 2:00 PM, within the vivarium's dark phase. After each 4-h session, the animals were returned to their home cages until the next day. A total of 27 adult male and female C57BL/6 mice with a mean age of ∼6 months were used in experiments, but the data from one mouse were excluded because the mouse did not interact with the FED3 device. All procedures were approved by the University of Michigan Committee on the Use and Care of Animals.

Procedure

Animals were injected intraperitoneally with either saline to function as a control (N = 14), or 25CN-NBOH (N = 12 mice) a 5-HT2A receptor agonist, purchased from Tocris Bioscience, at a dose of 10 mg/kg (a dose previously shown to induce psychedelic-like effects in mice (44)), dissolved in sterile saline and brought up to a total volume less than 1% of the mouse weight. To allow the blinding of the main experimenter, these injections were done by another experimenter, and the main experimenter remained blinded until after the protocol had been completed. After injection, the animals were left to rest for 24 h in their home cage before beginning an 85% free feeding weight schedule for 2 days. During those 2 days, a few reward pellets were dropped into each animal's cage. This was done to ensure proper food motivation and acclimation to the pellets.

After 2 days of food restriction, animals were introduced to a training period to acclimatize to the FED3. After this point, most of the daily food was obtained through the FED3 device; chow was added to home cages supplementally as needed to maintain at least 85% free-feeding weight. However, most mice returned to free-feeding weight over the course of the full protocol. For 2 days, the animals underwent the habituation phase of the protocol, in which the animals were introduced to the FED3 and experimentation cages. Over the course of two separate 4-h sessions, the FED3 automatically delivered a reward pellet every 4 min and in response to any pokes to either nose poke hole. After the 2 days of habituation, the animals began fixed-ratio 1 (FR1) training phase in which a reward pellet would be delivered any time the mouse poked the left nose poke hole. Similarly, these sessions were 4 h long each and took place over the course of 3 days.

After the 5 days of training were completed, the mouse was then introduced to the sequential fixed-ratio 2 (SEQFR2)-forward and SEQFR2-reversal phases. To receive a reward pellet in the SEQFR2-forward phase, the animal must poke the left nose poke hole followed by the right nose poke hole within 30 s. Should the animal poke the right hole in isolation, the left hole twice in a row, or not follow a left poke with a right poke, the device entered a 10-s timeout phase in which no further pokes would be registered and any nose pokes during this timeout period were ignored. Like all other sessions, these sessions lasted for 4 h each over the course of 6 days. After those 6 days elapsed, the SEQFR2 rule was reversed, meaning the animal had to poke the right nose poke hole first followed by the left to receive a reward pellet. Like the forward phase, the reversal phase lasted for 6 days. After the protocol was completed, mice were returned to their normal feeding schedules, cages were cleaned and sanitized with Liquinox lab detergent, and FED3s were sanitized with 70% ethanol. The experimental timeline and protocol overviews are summarized in Figure 1.

Behavioral Analysis

In the SEQFR2 task, poke efficiency is the main measure of performance. This was calculated by finding the proportion of pellets dispensed out of all pokes carried out by the animal in each hour. As additional metrics of behavioral performance, we examined cumulative pellets dispensed over the course of each phase, as well as the proportion of correct trials initiated out of all trials (“percent correct”). Together, these three metrics reflect the absolute performance animals over the course of the task (cumulative pellets), as well performance relative to the amount of engagement with the device (poke efficiency and percent correct).

Statistics

Statistical procedures were performed with Prism GraphPad (version 10.3.0) and R. We conducted multiple linear regression analyses as our primary statistical method, which has also been used as the analysis method in other reversal learning paradigms (6668). To ensure the robustness of our results, we conducted additional analyses by calculating each individual animal's regression curve and conducting Welch's one-sided t-tests to compare the saline and NBOH cohorts. Further statistical test information and significance are provided in the results section and figure legends.

Data Availability

Data generated in this study is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all the members of the Ahmed lab for helpful discussions related to this project.

Author Contributions

Elizabeth Brouns acted as an investigator for this study. She conducted all the data collection, developed the task, contributed significantly to the writing of the final manuscript, generated figures, conducted preliminary analyses, managed data, and planned all animals run on this task. Tyler Ekins acted as an investigator for this study. He injected all mice to keep the main experimenter blinded, ran final statistical analyses, generated figures, and contributed significantly to the writing of this manuscript. Omar Ahmed was the principal investigator and senior author of this study, designed the study, developed the task, helped with planning the animals run on the task, contributed to figure design, and contributed significantly to the writing and editing of this manuscript.

Funding Sources

This work was supported by: NIH R01MH129282; NIH P50NS123067, NIH T32DA007268, University of Michigan Eisenberg Family Depression Center Eisenberg Scholar Award.

Author Disclosures

The authors declare no competing financial interest.

Supplementary Material

References

  • 1.

    Bogenschutz MP
    ,
    Forcehimes AA
    ,
    Pommy JA
    ,
    Wilcox CE
    ,
    Barbosa P
    ,
    Strassman RJ
    . Psilocybin-assisted treatment for alcohol dependence: A proof-of-concept study. J Psychopharmacol. 2015;29(
    3
    ):28999. DOI: 10.1177/0269881114565144. PMID: 25586396

  • 2.

    Johnson MW
    ,
    Garcia-Romeu A
    ,
    Griffiths RR
    . Long-term follow-up of psilocybin-facilitated smoking cessation. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. 2017;43(
    1
    ):5560. DOI: 10.3109/00952990.2016.1170135. PMID: 27441452; PMCID: PMC5641975

  • 3.

    Carhart-Harris RL
    ,
    Bolstridge M
    ,
    Rucker J
    ,
    Day CMJ
    ,
    Erritzoe D
    ,
    Kaelen M
    , et al. Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: an open-label feasibility study. Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(
    7
    ):61927. DOI: 10.1016/S2215-0366(16)30065-7. PMID: 27210031

  • 4.

    Sanches RF
    ,
    de Lima Osório F
    ,
    Dos Santos RG
    ,
    Macedo LRH
    ,
    Maia-de-Oliveira JP
    ,
    Wichert-Ana L
    , et al. Antidepressant effects of a single dose of Ayahuasca in patients with recurrent depression: A SPECT Study. J Clin Psychopharmacol. 2016;36(
    1
    ):7781. DOI: 10.1097/JCP.0000000000000436. PMID: 26650973

  • 5.

    Carhart-Harris RL
    ,
    Roseman L
    ,
    Bolstridge M
    ,
    Demetriou L
    ,
    Pannekoek JN
    ,
    Wall MB
    , et al. Psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression: fMRI-measured brain mechanisms. Sci Rep. 2017;7(
    1
    ):13187. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-017-13282-7. PMID: 29030624; PMCID: PMC5640601

  • 6.

    Carhart-Harris RL
    ,
    Bolstridge M
    ,
    Day CMJ
    ,
    Rucker J
    ,
    Watts R
    ,
    Erritzoe DE
    , et al. Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: six-month follow-up. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2018;235(
    2
    ):399408. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-017-4771-x. PMID: 29119217; PMCID: PMC5813086

  • 7.

    Stroud JB
    ,
    Freeman TP
    ,
    Leech R
    ,
    Hindocha C
    ,
    Lawn W
    ,
    Nutt DJ
    , et al. Psilocybin with psychological support improves emotional face recognition in treatment-resistant depression. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2018;235(
    2
    ):45966. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-017-4754-y. PMID: 29085980; PMCID: PMC5813058

  • 8.

    Roseman L
    ,
    Nutt DJ
    ,
    Carhart-Harris RL
    . Quality of acute psychedelic experience predicts therapeutic efficacy of psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Front Pharmacol. 2018;8:974. DOI: 10.3389/fphar.2017.00974. PMID: 29387009; PMCID: PMC5776504

  • 9.

    Palhano-Fontes F
    ,
    Barreto D
    ,
    Onias H
    ,
    Andrade KC
    ,
    Novaes MM
    ,
    Pessoa JA
    , et al. Rapid antidepressant effects of the psychedelic ayahuasca in treatment-resistant depression: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychol Med. 2019;49(
    4
    ):65563. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291718001356. PMID: 29903051; PMCID: PMC6378413

  • 10.

    Carhart-Harris R
    ,
    Giribaldi B
    ,
    Watts R
    ,
    Baker-Jones M
    ,
    Murphy-Beiner A
    ,
    Murphy R
    , et al. Trial of psilocybin versus Escitalopram for depression. New Engl J Med. 2021;384(
    15
    ):140211. DOI: 10.1056/NEJMoa2032994. PMID: 33852780

  • 11.

    Davis AK
    ,
    Barrett FS
    ,
    May DG
    ,
    Cosimano MP
    ,
    Sepeda ND
    ,
    Johnson MW
    , et al. Effects of psilocybin-assisted therapy on major depressive disorder: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry. 2021;78(
    5
    ):4819. DOI: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.3285. PMID: 33146667; PMCID: PMC7643046

  • 12.

    Goodwin GM
    ,
    Aaronson ST
    ,
    Alvarez O
    ,
    Arden PC
    ,
    Baker A
    ,
    Bennett JC
    , et al. Single-dose psilocybin for a treatment-resistant episode of major depression. N Engl J Med. 2022;387(
    18
    ):163748. DOI: 10.1056/NEJMoa2206443. PMID: 36322843

  • 13.

    Doss MK
    ,
    Považan M
    ,
    Rosenberg MD
    ,
    Sepeda ND
    ,
    Davis AK
    ,
    Finan PH
    , et al. Psilocybin therapy increases cognitive and neural flexibility in patients with major depressive disorder. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11(
    1
    ):574. DOI: 10.1038/s41398-021-01706-y. PMID: 34750350; PMCID: PMC8575795

  • 14.

    Lyons T
    ,
    Spriggs M
    ,
    Kerkelä L
    ,
    Rosas F
    ,
    Roseman L
    ,
    Mediano P
    , et al. Human brain changes after first psilocybin use. bioRxiv. 2024;2024.10.11.617955.

  • 15.

    Cook SC
    ,
    Wellman CL
    . Chronic stress alters dendritic morphology in rat medial prefrontal cortex. J Neurobiol. 2004;60(
    2
    ):23648. DOI: 10.1002/neu.20025. PMID: 15266654

  • 16.

    Bondi CO
    ,
    Rodriguez G
    ,
    Gould GG
    ,
    Frazer A
    ,
    Morilak DA
    . Chronic unpredictable stress induces a cognitive deficit and anxiety-like behavior in rats that is prevented by Chronic antidepressant drug treatment. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(
    2
    ):32031. DOI: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301410. PMID: 17406647

  • 17.

    Chiba S
    ,
    Numakawa T
    ,
    Ninomiya M
    ,
    Richards MC
    ,
    Wakabayashi C
    ,
    Kunugi H
    . Chronic restraint stress causes anxiety- and depression-like behaviors, downregulates glucocorticoid receptor expression, and attenuates glutamate release induced by brain-derived neurotrophic factor in the prefrontal cortex. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2012;39(
    1
    ):1129. DOI: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2012.05.018. PMID: 22664354

  • 18.

    McKlveen JM
    ,
    Morano RL
    ,
    Fitzgerald M
    ,
    Zoubovsky S
    ,
    Cassella SN
    ,
    Scheimann JR
    , et al. Chronic stress increases prefrontal inhibition: A mechanism for stress-induced prefrontal dysfunction. Biol Psychiatry. 2016;80(
    10
    ):75464. DOI: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.03.2101. PMID: 27241140; PMCID: PMC5629635

  • 19.

    Qiao H
    ,
    Li M-X
    ,
    Xu C
    ,
    Chen H-B
    ,
    An S-C
    ,
    Ma X-M
    . Dendritic spines in depression: what we learned from animal models. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:8056370. DOI: 10.1155/2016/8056370. PMID: 26881133; PMCID: PMC4736982

  • 20.

    Negrón-Oyarzo I
    ,
    Aboitiz F
    ,
    Fuentealba P
    . Impaired functional connectivity in the prefrontal cortex: A mechanism for chronic stress-induced neuropsychiatric disorders. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:7539065. DOI: 10.1155/2016/7539065. PMID: 26904302; PMCID: PMC4745936

  • 21.

    Jett JD
    ,
    Bulin SE
    ,
    Hatherall LC
    ,
    McCartney CM
    ,
    Morilak DA
    . Deficits in cognitive flexibility induced by chronic unpredictable stress are associated with impaired glutamate neurotransmission in the rat medial prefrontal cortex. Neuroscience. 2017;346:28497.

  • 22.

    Wellman CL
    ,
    Bollinger JL
    ,
    Moench KM
    . Effects of stress on the structure and function of the medial prefrontal cortex: Insights from animal models. Int Rev Neurobiol. 2020;150:12953. DOI: 10.1016/bs.irn.2019.11.007. PMID: 32204829; PMCID: PMC9483990

  • 23.

    Ly C
    ,
    Greb AC
    ,
    Cameron LP
    ,
    Wong JM
    ,
    Barragan EV
    ,
    Wilson PC
    , et al. Psychedelics promote structural and functional neural plasticity. Cell Rep. 2018;23:317082. DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2018.05.022. PMID: 29898390; PMCID: PMC6082376

  • 24.

    Shao L-X
    ,
    Liao C
    ,
    Gregg I
    ,
    Davoudian PA
    ,
    Savalia NK
    ,
    Delagarza K
    , et al. Psilocybin induces rapid and persistent growth of dendritic spines in frontal cortex in vivo. Neuron. 2021;109(
    16
    ):253544.e4. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.06.008. PMID: 34228959; PMCID: PMC8376772

  • 25.

    Cameron LP
    ,
    Tombari RJ
    ,
    Lu J
    ,
    Pell AJ
    ,
    Hurley ZQ
    ,
    Ehinger Y
    , et al. A non-hallucinogenic psychedelic analogue with therapeutic potential. Nature. 2021;589(
    7842
    ):4749. DOI: 10.1038/s41586-020-3008-z. PMID: 33299186; PMCID: PMC7874389

  • 26.

    de la Fuente RM
    ,
    Zhu B
    ,
    Guevara CA
    ,
    Naler LB
    ,
    Saunders JM
    ,
    Zhou Z
    , et al. Prolonged epigenomic and synaptic plasticity alterations following single exposure to a psychedelic in mice. Cell Rep. 2021;37(
    3
    ):109836. DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2021.109836. PMID: 34686347; PMCID: PMC8582597

  • 27.

    Ekins TG
    ,
    Brooks I
    ,
    Kailasa S
    ,
    Rybicki-Kler C
    ,
    Jedrasiak-Cape I
    ,
    Donoho E
    , et al. Cellular rules underlying psychedelic control of prefrontal pyramidal neurons. bioRxiv. 2023;2023.10.20.563334. DOI: 10.1101/2023.10.20.563334. PMID: 37961554; PMCID: PMC10634703

  • 28.

    Cameron LP
    ,
    Patel SD
    ,
    Vargas MV
    ,
    Barragan EV
    ,
    Saeger HN
    ,
    Warren HT
    , et al. 5-HT2ARs mediate therapeutic behavioral effects of psychedelic tryptamines. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2023;14(
    3
    ):3518. DOI: 10.1021/acschemneuro.2c00718. PMID: 36630260; PMCID: PMC9939288

  • 29.

    Jefferson SJ
    ,
    Gregg I
    ,
    Dibbs M
    ,
    Liao C
    ,
    Wu H
    ,
    Davoudian PA
    , et al. 5-MeO-DMT modifies innate behaviors and promotes structural neural plasticity in mice. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2023;48(
    9
    ):125766. DOI: 10.1038/s41386-023-01572-w. PMID: 37015972; PMCID: PMC10354037

  • 30.

    Vargas MV
    ,
    Dunlap LE
    ,
    Dong C
    ,
    Carter SJ
    ,
    Tombari RJ
    ,
    Jami SA
    , et al. Psychedelics promote neuroplasticity through the activation of intracellular 5-HT2A receptors. Science. 2023;379(
    6633
    ):7006. DOI: 10.1126/science.adf0435. PMID: 36795823; PMCID: PMC10108900

  • 31.

    Shao LX
    ,
    Liao C
    ,
    Davoudian PA
    ,
    Savalia NK
    ,
    Jiang Q
    ,
    Wojtasiewicz C
    , et al. Psilocybin's lasting action requires pyramidal cell types and 5-HT2A receptors. Nature. 2025; DOI: 10.1038/s41586-025-08813-6. PMID: 40175553

  • 32.

    Miller EK
    ,
    Cohen JD
    . An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2001;24:167202. DOI: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. PMID: 11283309

  • 33.

    Kim C
    ,
    Johnson NF
    ,
    Cilles SE
    ,
    Gold BT
    . Common and distinct mechanisms of cognitive flexibility in prefrontal cortex. J Neurosci. 2011;31(
    13
    ):47719. DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5923-10.2011. PMID: 21451015; PMCID: PMC3086290

  • 34.

    Uddin LQ
    . Cognitive and behavioural flexibility: neural mechanisms and clinical considerations. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2021;22(
    3
    ):16779. DOI: 10.1038/s41583-021-00428-w. PMID: 33536614; PMCID: PMC7856857

  • 35.

    Grant JE
    ,
    Chamberlain SR
    . Impaired cognitive flexibility across psychiatric disorders. CNS Spectr. 2023;28(
    6
    ):68892. DOI: 10.1017/S1092852923002237. PMID: 37114560

  • 36.

    Guarino A
    ,
    Favieri F
    ,
    Boncompagni I
    ,
    Agostini F
    ,
    Cantone M
    ,
    Casagrande M
    . Executive functions in Alzheimer disease: A systematic review. Front Aging Neurosci. 2019;10:437. DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437. PMID: 30697157; PMCID: PMC6341024

  • 37.

    Bissonette GB
    ,
    Powell EM
    . Reversal learning and attentional set-shifting in mice. Neuropharmacology. 2012;62(
    3
    ):116874. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2011.03.011. PMID: 21439304; PMCID: PMC3130808

  • 38.

    Heisler JM
    ,
    Morales J
    ,
    Donegan JJ
    ,
    Jett JD
    ,
    Redus L
    ,
    O'Connor JC
    . The attentional set shifting task: A measure of cognitive flexibility in mice. J Vis Exp. 2015;(
    96
    ):51944. DOI: 10.3791/51944. PMID: 25741905; PMCID: PMC4354620

  • 39.

    Hamilton DA
    ,
    Brigman JL
    . Behavioral flexibility in rats and mice: Contributions of distinct frontocortical regions. Genes Brain Behav. 2015;14(
    1
    ):421. DOI: 10.1111/gbb.12191. PMID: 25561028; PMCID: PMC4482359

  • 40.

    Remmelink E
    ,
    Smit AB
    ,
    Verhage M
    ,
    Loos M
    . Measuring discrimination- and reversal learning in mouse models within 4 days and without prior food deprivation. Learn Mem. 2016;23(
    11
    ):6607. DOI: 10.1101/lm.042085.116. PMID: 27918287; PMCID: PMC5066605

  • 41.

    Hansen M
    ,
    Phonekeo K
    ,
    Paine JS
    ,
    Leth-Petersen S
    ,
    Begtrup M
    ,
    Bräuner-Osborne H
    , et al. Synthesis and structure–activity relationships of N-benzyl phenethylamines as 5-HT2A/2C agonists. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2014;5(
    3
    ):2439. DOI: 10.1021/cn400216u. PMID: 24397362; PMCID: PMC3963123

  • 42.

    Fantegrossi WE
    ,
    Gray BW
    ,
    Bailey JM
    ,
    Smith DA
    ,
    Hansen M
    ,
    Kristensen JL
    . Hallucinogen-like effects of 2-([2-(4-cyano-2,5-dimethoxyphenyl) ethylamino]methyl)phenol (25CN-NBOH), a novel N-benzylphenethylamine with 100-fold selectivity for 5-HT2A receptors, in mice. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2015;232(
    6
    ):103947. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-014-3739-3. PMID: 25224567; PMCID: PMC4339409

  • 43.

    Jensen AA
    ,
    McCorvy JD
    ,
    Petersen SL
    ,
    Bundgaard C
    ,
    Liebscher G
    ,
    Kenakin TP
    , et al. Detailed characterization of the in vitro pharmacological and pharmacokinetic properties of N-(2-hydroxybenzyl)-2,5-dimethoxy-4-cyanophenylethylamine (25CN-NBOH), a highly selective and brain-penetrant 5-HT2A receptor agonist. J Pharmacol Exp Ther. 2017;361(
    3
    ):44153. DOI: 10.1124/jpet.117.239905. PMID: 28360333

  • 44.

    Halberstadt AL
    ,
    Sindhunata IS
    ,
    Scheffers K
    ,
    Flynn AD
    ,
    Sharp RF
    ,
    Geyer MA
    , et al. Effect of 5-HT2A and 5-HT2C receptors on temporal discrimination by mice. Neuropharmacology. 2016;107:36475. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2016.03.038. PMID: 27020041; PMCID: PMC5403251

  • 45.

    Buchborn T
    ,
    Lyons T
    ,
    Knöpfel T
    . Tolerance and tachyphylaxis to head twitches induced by the 5-HT2A agonist 25CN-NBOH in mice. Front Pharmacol. 2018;9:17. DOI: 10.3389/fphar.2018.00017. PMID: 29467649; PMCID: PMC5808243

  • 46.

    Odland AU
    ,
    Kristensen JL
    ,
    Andreasen JT
    . The selective 5-HT2A receptor agonist 25CN-NBOH does not affect reversal learning in mice. Behav Pharmacol.2021;32:44852. DOI: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000626. PMID: 33595957

  • 47.

    Holze F
    ,
    Singh N
    ,
    Liechti ME
    ,
    D'Souza DC
    . Serotonergic psychedelics: A comparative review of efficacy, safety, pharmacokinetics, and binding profile. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2024;9:47289. DOI: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2024.01.007. PMID: 38301886

  • 48.

    Poulie CBM
    ,
    Jensen AA
    ,
    Halberstadt AL
    ,
    Kristensen JL
    . DARK classics in chemical neuroscience: NBOMes. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2019;11:386069. DOI: 10.1021/acschemneuro.9b00528. PMID: 31657895; PMCID: PMC9191638

  • 49.

    Märcher-Rørsted E
    ,
    Jensen AA
    ,
    Kristensen JL
    . 25CN-NBOH: A selective agonist for in vitro and in vivo investigations of the serotonin 2A receptor. ChemMedChem. 2021;16(
    21
    ):326370. DOI: 10.1002/cmdc.202100395. PMID: 34288515

  • 50.

    Gargiulo AT
    ,
    Hu J
    ,
    Ravaglia IC
    ,
    Hawks A
    ,
    Li X
    ,
    Sweasy K
    , et al. Sex differences in cognitive flexibility are driven by the estrous cycle and stress-dependent. Front Behav Neurosci. 2022;16:958301. DOI: 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.958301. PMID: 35990724; PMCID: PMC9386277

  • 51.

    Fisher EL
    ,
    Smith R
    ,
    Conn K
    ,
    Corcoran AW
    ,
    Milton LK
    ,
    Hohwy J
    , et al. Psilocybin increases optimistic engagement over time: computational modelling of behaviour in rats. Transl Psychiatry. 2024;14:394. DOI: 10.1038/s41398-024-03103-7. PMID: 39349428; PMCID: PMC11442808

  • 52.

    Conn K
    ,
    Milton LK
    ,
    Huang K
    ,
    Munguba H
    ,
    Ruuska J
    ,
    Lemus MB
    , et al. Psilocybin restrains activity-based anorexia in female rats by enhancing cognitive flexibility: Contributions from 5-HT1A and 5-HT2A receptor mechanisms. Mol Psychiatry. 2024;29:3291304. DOI: 10.1038/s41380-024-02575-9. PMID: 38678087; PMCID: PMC11449803

  • 53.

    Amodeo DA
    ,
    Hassan O
    ,
    Klein L
    ,
    Halberstadt AL
    ,
    Powell SB
    . Acute serotonin 2A receptor activation impairs behavioral flexibility in mice. Behav Brain Res.2020;395:112861. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112861. PMID: 32814148

  • 54.

    Pacheco AT
    ,
    Olson RJ
    ,
    Garza G
    ,
    Moghaddam B
    . Acute psilocybin enhances cognitive flexibility in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology.2023;48:101120. DOI: 10.1101/2023.01.09.523291. PMID: 36712091; PMCID: PMC9881983

  • 55.

    Pokorny T
    ,
    Duerler P
    ,
    Seifritz E
    ,
    Vollenweider FX
    ,
    Preller KH
    . LSD acutely impairs working memory, executive functions, and cognitive flexibility, but not risk- based decision-making. Psychol Med. 2020;50:225564. DOI: 10.1017/S0033291719002393. PMID: 31500679

  • 56.

    Frederick DL
    ,
    Gillam MP
    ,
    Lensing S
    ,
    Paule MG
    . Acute effects of LSD on Rhesus monkey operant test battery performance. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1997;57:63341. DOI: 10.1016/s0091-3057(96)00469-8. PMID: 9258988

  • 57.

    Bouso JC
    ,
    Fábregas JM
    ,
    Antonijoan RM
    ,
    Rodríguez-Fornells A
    ,
    Riba J
    . Acute effects of ayahuasca on neuropsychological performance: Differences in executive function between experienced and occasional users. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;230:41524. DOI: 10.1007/s00213-013-3167-9. PMID: 23793226

  • 58.

    Šabanović M
    ,
    Lazari A
    ,
    Blanco-Pozo M
    ,
    Tisca C
    ,
    Tachrount M
    ,
    Martins-Bach AB
    , et al. Lasting dynamic effects of the psychedelic 2,5-dimethoxy-4- iodoamphetamine ((±)-DOI) on cognitive flexibility. Mol Psychiatry. 2024;29:181023. DOI: 10.1038/s41380-024-02439-2. PMID: 38321122; PMCID: PMC11371652

  • 59.

    Faillace LA
    ,
    Vourlekis A
    ,
    Szara S
    . Clinical evaluation of some hallucinogenic tryptamine derivatives. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1967;145:30613. DOI: 10.1097/00005053-196710000-00005. PMID: 6076017

  • 60.

    González-Maeso J
    ,
    Weisstaub NV
    ,
    Zhou M
    ,
    Chan P
    ,
    Ivic L
    ,
    Ang R
    , et al. Hallucinogens recruit specific cortical 5-HT2A receptor-mediated signaling pathways to affect behavior. Neuron. 2007;53:43952. DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.01.008. PMID: 17270739

  • 61.

    Lewis V
    ,
    Bonniwell EM
    ,
    Lanham JK
    ,
    Ghaffari A
    ,
    Sheshbaradaran H
    ,
    Cao AB
    , et al. A non-hallucinogenic LSD analog with therapeutic potential for mood disorders. Cell Rep. 2023;42:112203. DOI: 10.1016/j.celrep.2023.112203. PMID: 36884348; PMCID: PMC10112881

  • 62.

    Maramis MM
    ,
    Mahajudin MS
    ,
    Khotib J
    . Impaired cognitive flexibility and working memory precedes depression: A rat model to study depression. Neuropsychobiology. 2020;80(
    3
    ):22533. DOI: 10.1159/000508682. PMID: 32712605; PMCID: PMC8220918

  • 63.

    George SA
    ,
    Rodriguez-Santiago M
    ,
    Riley J
    ,
    Abelson JL
    ,
    Floresco SB
    ,
    Liberzon I
    . Alterations in cognitive flexibility in a rat model of post-traumatic stress disorder. Behav Brain Res. 2015;286:25664. DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.02.051. PMID: 25746511

  • 64.

    Guarino A
    ,
    Forte G
    ,
    Giovannoli J
    ,
    Casagrande M
    . Executive functions in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review on motor and cognitive inhibition, conflict control and cognitive flexibility. Aging Ment Health. 2019;24(
    7
    ):102845. DOI: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1584785. PMID: 30938193

  • 65.

    Tyler E
    ,
    Kravitz L
    . Mouse at feeder. DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.3926026.

  • 66.

    D'Agostini M
    ,
    Burger AM
    ,
    Franssen M
    ,
    Claes N
    ,
    Weymar M
    ,
    von Leupoldt A
    , et al. Effects of transcutaneous auricular vagus nerve stimulation on reversal learning, tonic pupil size, salivary alpha-amylase, and cortisol. Psychophysiology. 2021;58(
    10
    ):e13885. DOI: 10.1111/psyp.13885. PMID: 34245461

  • 67.

    Grossman CD
    ,
    Bari BA
    ,
    Cohen JY
    . Serotonin neurons modulate learning rate through uncertainty. Curr Biol. 2022;32(
    3
    ):58699.e7. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2021.12.006. PMID: 34936883; PMCID: PMC8825708

  • 68.

    Chidambaram S
    ,
    Wintergerst S
    ,
    Kacelnik A
    ,
    Nachev V
    ,
    Winter Y
    . Serial reversal learning in nectar-feeding bats. Anim Cogn. 2024;27(
    1
    ):24. DOI: 10.1007/s10071-024-01836-y. PMID: 38451365; PMCID: PMC10920430

  • Download PDF
Copyright: © The Author(s), 2025. This article is under exclusive and permanent license to Genomic Press
pdf
pdf
Figure 1.
Figure 1.

Experimental timeline and overview. (A) Experimental timeline (65). (B) Schematic of the SEQFR2-forward protocol. Mice have to sequentially poke left and then right within 30 s to earn a reward pellet. (C) Schematic of the SEQFR2-reversal protocol. Mice now are required to poke right and then left within 30 s to get a reward pellet.


Figure 2.
Figure 2.

Single-dose psychedelic treatment induces a lasting reversal learning enhancement. (A) Group forward phase poke efficiency, with no significant effect of NBOH treatment. Each day has 4 points plotted corresponding to each hour of the 4-h per day sessions. (B) Group forward phase percentage of correct trials, indicating no significant effect of NBOH treatment. (C) NBOH treatment significantly increased the number of reward pellets dispensed during the forward phase. (D) NBOH treatment significantly increased poke efficiency during the reversal phase compared to saline injection, indicating enhanced cognitive flexibility. (E) NBOH increases the percentage of correct trials. (F) NBOH treatment significantly increased the number of reward pellets dispensed during the reversal phase. Shaded regions represent standard error of the mean (SEM), linear regressions shown in pink for NBOH and black for saline; ns, not significant; **p < 0.01; ****p < 0.0001.


Figure 3.
Figure 3.

Lasting psychedelic enhancement of reversal learning ability in male and female mice. (Top) Forward and reversal phase changes to poke efficiency indicating NBOH treatment significantly improves reversal learning in female mice weeks after a single dose. (Bottom) Forward and reversal phase changes to poke efficiency in male mice indicating NBOH treatment significantly improves reversal learning weeks after a single dose. Shaded regions represent standard error of the mean (SEM), linear regressions shown in pink for NBOH and black for saline; ns, not significant; **p < 0.01; ****p < 0.0001.


Contributor Notes

Corresponding Author: Omar J. Ahmed, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, 530 Church St., Ann Arbor, MI 48109. E-mail: ojahmed@umich.edu

Publisher's note: Genomic Press maintains a position of impartiality and neutrality regarding territorial assertions represented in published materials and affiliations of institutional nature. As such, we will use the affiliations provided by the authors, without editing them. Such use simply reflects what the authors submitted to us and it does not indicate that Genomic Press supports any type of territorial assertions.

Received: Nov 18, 2024
Accepted: Apr 02, 2025